Skip to main content

Perceived healthiness of sugary drinks and related social norms among adults in five countries: evidence from the International Food Policy Study

Abstract

Background

A better understanding of correlates of sugary drink consumption is essential to inform public health interventions. This study examined differences in perceived healthiness of sugary drinks and related social norms between countries, over time, and sociodemographic groups and associations with sugary drink intake.

Methods

This study used annual cross-sectional data from the International Food Policy Study from 2018 to 2021 in Australia, Canada, the United Kingdom, the United States, and Mexico. Analyses examined perceived healthiness of eight beverage types and two types of perceived social norms (descriptive, injunctive) that discourage sugary drink consumption. The 24-item Beverage Frequency Questionnaire was used to estimate beverage intake in the past 7 days. Logistic regression models examined trends over time in odds of perceiving each beverage type as unhealthy and agreeing with social norms discouraging sugary drink consumption, across countries and sociodemographic characteristics. Negative binomial regressions examined associations between perceived healthiness, social norms and consumption.

Results

Energy drinks, regular soft drinks, and diet soft drinks were most frequently perceived as unhealthy in all countries, while water and 100% juice were least frequently perceived as unhealthy. Participants in Mexico had higher odds of perceiving 100% juice, chocolate milk, and iced tea as unhealthy in 2021 compared to 2018 (AOR = 1.71 99%CI 1.10–2.64; AOR = 2.69, 99%CI 1.70–4.26; AOR = 1.79, 99%CI 1.15–2.76; respectively), with little change in other countries. Agreement with social norms discouraging consumption of sugary drinks was higher in Mexico than in other countries. Trends in social norms over time were mostly stable, except in Mexico where participants had higher odds of agreeing with both norms in 2020 compared to 2018 (AOR = 1.27, 99%CI 1.09–1.48 for a descriptive norm and AOR = 1.27 99%CI 1.09–1.49 for an injunctive norm). In most countries, perceiving a beverage as unhealthy and agreeing with social norms discouraging consumption of sugary drink were associated with lower sugary drink consumption, with varying strength of associations across countries and beverage types.

Conclusions

Shifts over time in social norms and perceived healthiness observed in Mexico and associations with intake of sugary drinks in most countries suggest that targeted interventions to change norms and perceptions could help reduce sugary drink consumption.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Consumption of sugary drinks is a significant public health concern as it contributes to excess free and added sugar intake [1,2,3,4]. High intakes of sugary drinks and free sugars are associated with an increased risk of noncommunicable diseases, such as cardiovascular disease [5], type 2 diabetes [6], cancer [7], and dental caries [8], with concomitant economic implications for the health care system [9]. Definitions of sugary drinks and sugar-sweetened beverages (SSBs) vary in the literature, but sugary drinks are usually defined as beverages containing free sugars, i.e., SSBs and 100% fruit juices [9, 10].

In recent years, several public health initiatives, including marketing restrictions [11], taxes [12], and mass media campaigns [13, 14], have targeted SSBs, in an effort to reduce consumption. However, while consumption of some beverage categories, such as traditional soft drinks and 100% juice, may be declining [15,16,17,18], the popularity of novel drink categories, including iced tea, flavored sweetened water, and energy drinks, continues to rise [15, 17,18,19,20].

Research has identified several factors associated with SSB consumption, including sociodemographic characteristics such as younger age, male sex and gender, lower income and education, living with overweight and obesity [16, 21,22,23,24], and other dietary and environmental factors [22,23,24], while demographic and behavioral correlates of consumption for 100% juice are less consistent [16, 25, 26]. Other potentially important individual-level factors that are shaped by sociocultural and policy environments, including beliefs about sugary drinks, have been less frequently studied. For example, social norms are an important determinant of behavior [27, 28] and are central to some behavior change theories [29, 30]. Most studies on social norms have focused on descriptive norms (what others do) related to SSBs [31,32,33,34], and, to a lesser extent, on injunctive norms (perceived approval/disapproval of others) [34]. Overall, evidence suggests that perceiving that others drink SSBs is associated with higher individual intake [31,32,33,34]. Perceived healthiness of a food has been shown to influence dietary behaviors [35] and the limited studies available have demonstrated wide variations in perceptions of healthiness by beverage type [36,37,38]. However, differences in perceptions by sociodemographic characteristics, for a range of beverages, including novel drink categories more seldomly consumed, have not been explored. In addition, few studies have examined whether social norms and perceived healthiness, related specifically to sugary drinks, vary across countries with different food policy environments. Understanding the correlates of sugary drink consumption and how they vary across sociodemographic groups and countries could help develop targeted interventions to promote healthier beverage choices while addressing social inequalities.

This study aimed to determine: 1) how perceived healthiness and social norms toward sugary drinks vary over time, across sociodemographic groups and countries; and 2) how perceived healthiness and social norms are associated with sugary drink intake.

Methods

Study design

Cross-sectional survey data were analyzed from the 2018-2021 waves of the International Food Policy (IFPS), which includes 5 countries with varying food environments, policies and cultures and thus provide unique opportunities for comparison [39]. Adults aged ≥ 18 years residing in Australia, Canada, Mexico, the United Kingdom (UK), and the United States (US) were recruited through the Nielsen Consumer Insights Global Panel and their partners’ panels using non-probability sampling methods. Invitations with unique survey access links were sent to a random sample of available panelists in each country. Quotas for age, sex, and language (when relevant) were applied to facilitate the recruitment of samples that were more representative of each country’s population, based on national census data in each country. Self-administered surveys were conducted online using Alchemer Online Survey Software. All participants provided consent prior to completing the survey and received remuneration following their panel’s usual incentive structure [40]. The study received ethical approval by a University of Waterloo Research Ethics Committee (REB #30829) and Université Laval Ethics Committee (#2021-318 Phase II A-2 R-2 / 11-12-2023).

Survey measures

Perceived healthiness of sugary drinks

Perceived healthiness of sugary drinks was assessed with the question: “In your opinion, how unhealthy or healthy is this type of drink?” with response options on a 7-point Likert scale (“Very unhealthy”, “Unhealthy”, “A little unhealthy”, “Neither healthy nor unhealthy”, “A little healthy”, “Healthy”, “Very healthy”). Participants were asked to respond for regular soft drinks (Coca-Cola™) and another randomly assigned branded beverage from the following 8 categories: diet soft drinks, 100% juice, energy drinks, water, specialty coffee, sports drinks, chocolate milk, or iced tea. Randomization to the second beverage category reviewed was conducted within each country sample. White milk was added in 2019 but was not analyzed in the current study. The product images shown to participants for the perceived healthiness questions can be found in Figure S1.These images matched brands and products commonly available in each country and remained the same across all waves, except in Mexico, where front-of-pack (FOP) warning labels were added in 2020, in line with national regulations [41]. Relative perceived healthiness was measured as the difference between the initial 7-point Likert scale healthiness rating of the regular soft drink and the rating of the additionally assigned drink.

Perceived social norms for sugary drinks

Participants were asked to respond to two statements, including “People important to me TRY NOT to drink SUGARY DRINKS” (a descriptive norm) and “People important to me THINK I SHOULD NOT drink SUGARY DRINKS” (an injunctive norm) (underlining and capital letters were used in the wording for emphasis), using a 5-point Likert scale, (“Strongly agree”, “Agree”, “Neither agree nor disagree”, “Disagree”, "Strongly disagree”) in addition to “Don’t know” and “Refuse to answer”. The underlined and capitalized words were changed for the 2021 survey, which may have influenced responses; therefore, social norm data from 2021 were excluded from analyses. Before answering the questions about social norms, participants were given the following definition: “Sugary drinks are drinks that contain added sugar, like pop, fruit drinks, sports drinks, energy drinks, chocolate milk, and specialty coffees that have added sugar”. Notably, this did not include 100% fruit juice, which is not consistent with most definitions of sugary drinks.

Sugary drink intake

Sugary drink consumption was estimated using the Beverage Frequency Questionnaire, which assesses consumption of 20 non-alcoholic and 4 alcoholic beverages and has demonstrated reasonable validity among Canadian young adults [42]. Participants were asked to report the number of drinks consumed with the following question: “During the past 7 days, how many drinks did you have in each category below?”. A sugary drink intake variable was created by summing the number of beverages consumed from each category that contained added sugars (regular soft drinks, sports drinks, energy drinks, chocolate/flavored milk, fruit drinks, flavored/vitamin waters, and sweetened smoothies/protein shakes/drinkable yogurts) but did not include 100% fruit juice, which was consistent with the definition of sugary drinks that was provided to participants. Specialty coffee and coffee/tea with sugar were not included in the grouped variable as these measures were changed over time and thus cannot be used for longitudinal comparisons. Alcoholic beverages were also not included in this analysis.

Participants who reported 70 to 100 beverages per week for a single drink category were recoded as 70 drinks/week (for each of those applicable drink categories). Participants were excluded from the BFQ analyses due to data quality concerns if they reported more than 100 beverages per week for a single drink category, there were inconsistencies in reporting numbers of drinks (e.g., reporting no drinks consumed all week, conflicting responses indicating no drinks consumed while also reporting specific drinks consumed, repetitive/sequential entries across all categories, non-numerical/nonsensical entries), missing information on the size of beverage consumed, the total volume consumed was reported as greater than 49 liters per week, and/or the total volume consumed, excluding water was reported as greater than 36 liters per week [40].

Sociodemographic characteristics

Sociodemographic variables selected a priori as covariates of interest included age, ethnicity, sex at birth, country, income, education, and the presence of a child under 18 years at home. Ethnicity was assessed using country-specific race/ethnicity categories and was analyzed as a binary variable (majority ethnicity/minority ethnicity) to facilitate comparisons across countries. Perceived income adequacy was assessed with the question, “Thinking about your total monthly income, how difficult or easy is it for you to make ends meet?”, with response options on a 5-point scale, from “Very difficult” to “Very easy”, which allows for better comparisons across countries than absolute household income [43]. Data were recoded as “Low income adequacy” (Very difficult/Difficult/Neither easy nor difficult to make ends meet) and “High income adequacy” (Easy/Very Easy to make ends meet). Education was assessed with the question, “What is the highest level of formal education that you have completed?”. Responses were categorized into three levels, including low (high school diploma or less), medium (technical diploma or some post-secondary qualifications), and high education (university degree or higher), to facilitate comparisons across countries.

For all variables (perceived healthiness, social norms, sociodemographic characteristics, and frequency of beverage consumption), participants who answered, “Refuse to answer” or “Don’t know” were considered missing and were excluded from all analyses. Participants who experienced a technical glitch in the survey programming (n=289), whereby they were not randomized and were asked the perceived healthiness question for all beverages, were also excluded, as were those who were randomly assigned to a glass of white milk in 2021, as this was not analyzed in the current study.

Statistical analysis

All analyses were performed with SAS Studio V.3.8. Data were weighted with post-stratification sample weights constructed using a raking algorithm with population estimates from census data in each country based on known population age, sex, region, ethnicity (except in Canada), and education level (except in Mexico) [40].Weights were rescaled to the final sample size. Descriptive estimates for sociodemographic, perceived healthiness, and social norms are reported as unadjusted weighted percentages or counts, by country and year which were calculated using SAS survey procedures.

In order to model factors that may be more likely to be associated with reduced consumption of sugary drinks (which is important from a public health perspective), continuous Likert scale responses for perceived healthiness were recoded into a binary variable of “Unhealthy” (very unhealthy/unhealthy/a little unhealthy) and “Not unhealthy” (neither healthy nor unhealthy/a little healthy/healthy/very healthy), and social normal responses were recoded into binary variables of “Agree” (strongly agree/agree) and “Not Agree” (neither agree nor disagree/disagree/strongly disagree). Sensitivity analyses were conducted to compare the use of a binary variable compared to a continuous scale and the pattern of results did not change (data not shown).

Logistic regressions weighted and adjusted for sociodemographic variables, including age, ethnicity, sex, perceived income adequacy, education, the presence of a child under 18 at home, and year were used to assess differences between countries in the odds of perceiving each beverage category as unhealthy (among the subsamples randomly assigned to each beverage), as well as the odds of agreeing with the descriptive and the injunctive norms. Similar regressions, stratified by country, examined differences by year and sociodemographic variables.

Adjusted negative binomial regressions, weighted and stratified by country and beverage, were used to examine the association between each correlate and sugary drink intake. For perceived healthiness, perception of each sugary beverage type was linked with intake of each drink. For example, models examined the association between perceived healthiness of regular soft drinks and the number of times a soft drink was consumed in the past week. Separate models were also used to examine the associations between each of the descriptive and injunctive norms and the number of times any sugary drink was consumed in the past seven days. Adjusted odds ratios (AORs) and incident risk ratios (IRRs) are reported with 99% confidence intervals to account for the large sample size and multiple comparisons examined. AORs and IRRs are interpreted using effect size (i.e., the magnitude of the association), as follows: <1.5 = very small, 1.5 to 2 = small, 2 to 3 = medium, > 3 = large [44].

Results

Sample characteristics

Of the 86 683 adults who completed the IFPS surveys, 14 182 (16.4%) participants had missing data for sociodemographic variables (n=1036 for perceived income adequacy, n=1045 for ethnicity, n=379 for education, n=179 for the presence of children at home), the two correlates of interest (n=2545 for perceived healthiness measures; n=6016 for social norms), or the beverage intake variables (n=8978; numbers may sum to greater than 14 182 due to missing data for multiple variables), and 2134 (2.5%) were randomized to conditions not examined in the current study. The final analytic sample included 70 367 participants (Australia=13 657; Canada=14 204; Mexico=14 220; UK=14 001; US=14 285). Weighted sample characteristics are shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Weighted sociodemographic characteristics across countries (2018–2021)a

Perceived healthiness of beverages

Figure 1 shows the unadjusted percentages of participants who reported each beverage as unhealthy, by country and year. Overall, the perception that a beverage was unhealthy was most common for energy drinks (range from 85% in the Mexico to 95% in Canada), regular soft drinks (range from 84% in Mexico to 94% in Australia and Canada), and diet soft drinks (range from 74% in the UK to 86% in Canada), and least common for water (about 2% in all countries) and 100% juice (range from 24% in the US to 51% in Mexico). Figure 2 shows the percentage of participants who rated each beverage category as “healthier” compared to the rating of regular soft drinks (e.g., a higher rating on the 7-point Likert scale). Most participants in all countries rated water, 100% juice, chocolate milk, iced tea, sports drinks, and specialty coffee as healthier than regular soft drinks. Figures detailing perceptions of each beverage type on the 7-point Likert scale are available in supplemental files (Figure S2).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Weighted percentages of participants reporting each beverage as unhealthy across all years combined and for each yeara. Legend: aParticipants were asked: “In your opinion, how unhealthy or healthy is this type of drink?” using a 7-point Likert-type scale, with response options “A little unhealthy”, “Unhealthy” and “Very unhealthy” categorized as “Unhealthy”. Error bars represent 99% confidence intervals. Abbreviations: AUS = Australia, CAN = Canada, MEX = Mexico, UK = United-Kingdom, US = United-States, All = All countries, ‘18 = 2018, ‘19 = 2019, ‘20 = 2020, ‘21 = 2021

Fig. 2
figure 2

Weighted percentages of participants who rated each beverage as healthiera than regular soft drinks (2018-2021)b. Legend:aParticipants were asked: “In your opinion, how unhealthy or healthy is this type of drink?” using a 7-point Likert-type scale for regular soft drinks and another randomly assigned beverage. bRelative perceived healthiness is the difference between perceived healthiness of regular soft drinks and of the additional beverage category to which the participant was randomized. Error bars represent 99% confidence intervals.   All countries   Australia   Canada   Mexico   UK   US

In adjusted models, there were mostly very small to moderate differences between countries in perceived healthiness of regular soft drinks, 100% juice, diet soft drinks, and energy drinks, with several large differences noted for sports drinks, chocolate milk, and iced tea (Table 2). Participants in Australia had higher odds of perceiving regular soft drinks, 100% juice, diet soft drinks, and iced tea, as unhealthy compared with those in the UK, the US, and Mexico (except 100% juice in Mexico), and higher odds of perceiving chocolate milk as unhealthy compared to all countries. Participants in the US had lower odds of perceiving all beverages, except specialty coffee, as unhealthy compared to those in Australia, Canada, and the UK (except diet soft drinks in the UK). Participants in Mexico had lower odds of perceiving regular soft drinks, energy drinks, sports drinks, and specialty coffee as unhealthy compared to those in Australia, Canada, and the UK, but greater odds of perceiving 100% juice as unhealthy compared to all countries, except Australia.

Table 2 Differences across countries in the perception of each beverage category as “unhealthy” (2018–2021)

In the UK, there were slightly lower odds of perceiving regular soft drinks as unhealthy in 2021, compared to 2019 and 2020 (Table 3). No other significant changes in perceived healthiness of regular soft drinks were observed over time.

Table 3 Perception that regular soft drinks are “unhealthy”, by country

Across all years, there was a moderate association with sex and a small association with having children at home, such that in all countries except Mexico, females and participants with no children at home had higher odds of perceiving regular soft drinks as unhealthy than males and those with children (Table 3). In Australia, the UK, and the US there was a small to moderate association with age, such that younger participants had lower odds of perceiving regular soft drinks as unhealthy than older age groups. There was also a very small association with income, ethnicity, and education such that participants with low income adequacy (US only), from an ethnic majority (Canada and US), and with high education levels (Canada, the UK and the US) had higher odds of perceiving regular soft drinks as unhealthy than those with high income adequacy, from an ethnic minority and with low education levels.

Results for perceived healthiness of other beverages can be found in supplementary files (Tables S1-S7). In Mexico, there were very small to moderate increases in the odds of perceiving 100% juice, chocolate milk, and iced tea as unhealthy in 2021 compared to 2018, and of perceiving specialty coffee as unhealthy in 2020 compared to 2018. Otherwise, trends over time were stable, except, in Australia where there was a small decrease in the odds of perceiving diet soft drinks as unhealthy between 2018 and 2021; in the US where there was a small increase in the odds of perceiving chocolate milk as unhealthy between 2018 and 2019; and in the UK where there was a very small decrease in the odds of perceiving chocolate milk as unhealthy between 2020 and 2021. In addition, mostly small to moderate differences were found by sex, level of education and presence of children at home, in some countries only. Females had higher odds of perceiving all beverages as unhealthy compared to males, participants with high education levels had higher odds of perceiving sports drinks and iced tea as unhealthy compared to less educated participants, and those with no children at home had higher odds of perceiving energy and sports drinks as unhealthy, compared to those with children at home. Small to large differences by ethnicity were also observed in some countries, such that participants from an ethnic majority had higher odds of perceiving energy drinks and specialty coffee as unhealthy compared to participants from an ethnic minority. Other differences were observed for education level, age, and presence of a child at home for some drink categories.

Social norms

The unadjusted percentages of participants who agreed with each type of social norm discouraging sugary drink consumption are shown in Fig. 3. Agreement ranged from 52% in the US and Canada to 62% in Mexico for the descriptive norm, and from 48% in the UK to 68% in Mexico for the injunctive norm. Additional figures detailing responses to social norms are available in supplemental files (Figure S3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Weighted percentages of participants agreeing with the descriptive and injunctive normsa. Legend: a Participants were asked “People important to me TRY NOT to drink SUGARY DRINKS” for the descriptive norm and “People important to me THINK I SHOULD NOT drink SUGARY DRINKS” for the injunctive norm, with response options “agree” and “strongly agree” categorized as “agree.” Error bars represent 99% confidence intervals. Abbreviations: AUS = Australia, CAN = Canada, MEX = Mexico, UK = United-Kingdom, US = United-States, All = All countries, ‘18 = 2018, ‘19 = 2019, ‘20 = 2020

There were mostly very small differences in the odds of agreeing with the descriptive norm statement between all countries, with the exceptions of no meaningful differences between the UK compared to Australia and the US compared to Canada. Overall, participants in Mexico had higher odds of agreeing with the descriptive norm compared to all other countries. Also, there were moderate differences between Mexico and the other countries for the injunctive norm, as agreement was higher in Mexico, and very small differences for all other comparisons, except for Canada compared to the UK and the US where no meaningful differences were observed (Table 4).

Table 4 Differences across countries in agreement with the descriptivea and injunctiveb norms, 2018–2020 (n = 54 438)

Tables 5 and 6 show results from stratified logistic regression models examining trends over time and by sociodemographic characteristics for the descriptive and injunctive norms, separately. In Mexico, there were very small increases in the odds of agreeing with both social norm statements between 2018 and 2020, and with the descriptive norm in 2020 compared to 2019. No differences over time were found in the other countries. Except in Mexico, age and education had small to moderate associations with social norms, such that the odds of agreeing with both norms increased with age and education levels. All other meaningful differences by sociodemographic characteristics had very small associations. Males had higher odds than females of agreeing with the injunctive norm in all countries and with the descriptive norm in Mexico only. Those classified as an ethnic minority had higher odds of agreeing with the injunctive and the descriptive norms, in all countries. High perceived income adequacy was associated with greater odds of agreement with the descriptive norm in all countries except Mexico, and with the injunctive norm in Australia. Finally, participants with children at home had higher odds of agreeing with the injunctive norm than participants with no children at home in Mexico, the UK, and the US and higher odds of agreement with the descriptive norm in Mexico and the US.

Table 5 Agreement with the descriptivea norm, by country
Table 6 Agreement with the injunctive norma, by country

Association between social norms, perceived healthiness, and intake

Estimates from adjusted negative binomial regression models across all years examining the association between each correlate and beverage intake are shown in Table 7. There were mostly small but meaningful associations between perceived healthiness and intake, with slightly larger associations for most beverages in the US and for sports and diet soft drinks in most countries. Participants who perceived regular soft drinks, 100% juice, and diet soft drinks as unhealthy had lower intakes of these beverages in all countries. Except in the UK, participants who perceived sports drinks as unhealthy had lower intakes of this beverage, and participants in Australia who perceived iced tea as unhealthy had lower intakes of iced tea. For chocolate milk, meaningful differences in intake according to perceived healthiness were found only in Mexico and the US.

Table 7 IRR of consuming each sugary drink type (for perceived healthiness) and any sugary drink (for social norms) in the past 7 days

Social norms had mostly very small associations with sugary drink intake. Agreement with the descriptive norm was associated with lower intake of sugary drinks in all countries, and agreement with the injunctive norm was associated with lower intake in Australia, Mexico, and the UK.

Discussion

This study examined correlates of sugary drink consumption using a repeat cross-sectional study design and identified differences between countries and over time. Overall, participants in Australia had greater odds of perceiving most sugary drinks as unhealthy, while participants in the US and Mexico had lower odds of perceiving them as unhealthy, compared to other countries. Over time, participants in Mexico had greater odds of perceiving 100% juice, chocolate milk, iced tea, and specialty coffee as unhealthy. In addition, agreement with the descriptive and the injunctive norms was higher in Mexico, than in other countries, and increased over time in Mexico only. Some differences were observed based on sociodemographic characteristics, including male participants having lower odds of perceiving sugary drinks as unhealthy, and females, younger individuals, those from an ethnic majority, and those with lower levels of education having lower odds of agreeing with both social norms examined. Our results also showed that perceiving a beverage as unhealthy and agreeing with social norms discouraging sugary drink consumption were generally associated with lower odds of consuming sugary drinks.

Perceived healthiness

Our findings reflect existing evidence that energy drinks, regular and diet soft drinks are widely perceived as unhealthy, while 100% juice and water are less likely to be perceived as unhealthy [36,37,38], and that 100% juice is perceived as healthier than regular soft drinks [45]. These findings may be attributed to the nutritional composition of these beverages, as consumers often perceive high sugar or high fructose corn syrup content, caffeine, and artificial sweeteners as concerning [36, 46], whereas they perceive fruit content and vitamins as positive attributes [36, 46, 47]. The perception of 100% juice may also reflect less consistent evidence on the health effects of this beverage [48, 49]. Indeed, 100% juices are still considered a serving of fruit in some, but not all, dietary guidelines [50], and are not consistently included in public health nutrition policies [51, 52], making them appear healthier to some consumers.

In Mexico, more participants perceived 100% juice, chocolate milk, specialty coffee, and iced tea, as unhealthy over time, which is in line with the implementation of the mandatory FOPs warning labels in 2020 (i.e., prominent stop signs on packaging to identify unhealthy beverages and warn consumers on the presence of caffeine and non-caloric sweeteners) [41]. Indeed, FOP labeling, and specifically “high in” symbols, has been shown to alter consumers' health perceptions of sugary drinks [53]. This suggests that perceptions are modifiable and highlights the potential for public health policy to influence beliefs.

Participants in the UK were less likely to perceive regular soft drinks as unhealthy in 2021 compared to 2019. Negative attitudes towards sugary drinks may have been heightened in the UK in 2018 as a result of the implementation of the soft drinks industry levy, a “tax” that was applied to manufacturers according to the amount of sugar in beverages to encourage reformulation and sugar reductions in beverages [54]. It is also possible that the reduction in the amount of sugar in beverages following the levy [55] may have led UK participants to perceive these drinks as “healthier”.

Our results also show differences in perceived healthiness across countries and sociodemographic groups. For example, higher proportions of participants in Australia perceived most beverages as unhealthy, which may reflect the country’s recent efforts to raise awareness of the negative effects of sugary drinks, such as the Live Lighter® and Rethink Sugary Drink campaigns [56, 57]. In addition, female participants, and those with high education levels tended to have higher odds of perceiving some sugary drinks as unhealthy. Previous research has indeed shown that women and those with higher education levels have greater nutrition knowledge [58] and identify health as a more important factor in food choices [59], which might explain these results. Perceptions also differed between adults with and without children at home. The strong preference of children for sugary drinks [60], the various marketing practices for children’s drinks aimed at parents [61], and the fact that parents are more exposed to sugary drinks through their children could result in parental confusion, potentially causing misperceptions about the healthfulness of sugary drinks. However, given that the data are cross-sectional, the direction of this association is unclear. Differences could also be explained by cognitive dissonance, such that parents who are more exposed to sugary drinks or who purchase more sugary drinks for their children change their perceived healthiness of these beverages to reduce feelings of discomfort or guilt.

Overall, our results show associations between perceiving a beverage as unhealthy and consuming less of that beverage, consistent with other studies [38, 62]. Associations were somewhat less prevalent for more “ambiguous” drinks (e.g., drinks that may be perceived as healthier due to some properties or marketing, but are in fact high in added sugar, such as sports drinks, chocolate milk, and iced tea) in the UK. This may in part be due to differences in food environments such as the marketing, price, and availability of these drinks in the UK.

The findings suggest that policies that influence perceptions of sugary drinks and target specific subgroups of the population could contribute to efforts to reduce sugary drink consumption. For instance, it may be of interest to target parental beliefs about sugary drinks, such as through targeted education campaigns, as parental misconceptions may influence which drinks they buy for their children [36, 63]. Sex-specific campaigns may also be relevant as males were less likely to perceive sugary drinks as unhealthy. Our findings may help inform which beverage categories should be included in regulations or policies, given, for example, that the consumption of sports drinks and chocolate milk is increasing [15, 17] and that they were perceived as “healthier” products in our study compared to products with similar levels of sugars. Our results also highlight the opportunity to better regulate food marketing practices that are likely to lead to misperceptions of healthiness, such as nutrition claims on beverages [64] or other persuasive advertising strategies commonly used in the marketing of sports, energy, and fruit drinks [65, 66].

Social norms

Approximately half of the sample agreed with the descriptive and injunctive norms indicating perceptions of negative attitudes toward sugary drinks in their social circles, with higher agreement in Mexico. This contrasts with previous studies published in 2011, and 2020 which showed norms in favor of SSB consumption in Mexico and Latino communities in the US [67,68,69]. Greater agreement may be partly explained by Mexico's more collectivist beliefs, with higher support of government policies and stronger negative reactions to norm violators than individualist countries [70, 71]. In the current study, agreement with each social norm in Mexico was higher in 2020 than in 2018, which may be related to the many recent governmental and non-governmental efforts to reduce the consumption of sugary drinks and raise awareness among the population, including governmental actions to improve food environments and major mobilizations of civil society organizations to promote public education campaigns on the health effects of sugary drinks and SSBs and generate support for public policies that address this issue [72,73,74]. These may have contributed to a change in norms in this country, especially as consumers in Mexico are known to be more aware of public campaigns to promote healthy eating than in other countries [75]. Differences in sociodemographic characteristics have also been found in other research examining social norms towards SSBs. For example, in our study, younger participants, especially those between 18 and 29 years old, were less likely to agree with both norms, consistent with another study on perceived societal disapproval of SSBs [34]. This could be explained by the role that SSBs play in young adults’ social interactions and social settings [76, 77], along with a change in the social acceptability of these drinks over time [78].

Despite higher agreement with both norms in Mexico, there was little association between norms and sugary drink intake in this country. The reduced association between social norms and behaviour may be related to strong cultural ties and historically high consumption of SSB in Mexico, which may weaken the link between beliefs and behaviors [67, 69, 79] or to the industry's strategies to counteract governmental efforts [80]. In all countries, sugary drink intake was more strongly associated with the descriptive norm compared to the injunctive norm, in line with previous studies suggesting that injunctive norms may be more closely related to the intention to engage in a behavior, whereas the descriptive norm is directly related to the behavior [28, 34]. Correcting misperceptions about descriptive norms by addressing how people perceive others’ sugary drink intake may therefore hold promise for reduction efforts [81]. More broadly, denormalizing sugary drink consumption, a technique that has been effective for smoking [82], may be particularly relevant for young people with lower education, as they were less likely to agree with both norms in our study and as their consumption is higher than older and more educated groups [83].

Strengths and limitations

This study has several strengths. First, the study used a large sample size, the same measures in five countries over four years, and allowed an in-depth examination of sociodemographic differences that may help identify which populations may benefit most from interventions or special consideration in population health approaches. In addition, a wide range of beverage categories that have rarely been examined individually were included, which may help inform policy decisions related to specific beverage categories. Moreover, this study will serve as a baseline for assessing the impact of future policies, such as FOP labeling, which will be introduced in Canada in 2026 [84], and is under consideration in the US [85].

This study is subject to limitations common to survey research. Participants were recruited using non-probability-based sampling and the results do not necessarily provide nationally representative estimates; however, the analyses were weighted to better represent the populations in each country. Moreover, participants with higher education were overrepresented in Mexico compared to other countries, suggesting the need to conduct research with populations with lower levels of education to confirm that the pattern of the results applies to them. However, the null association between education and perceived healthiness and social norms in Mexico suggests that selection bias may not have compromised the overall results. Also, the cross-sectional nature of these data does not allow for the establishment of causal relationships. The use of binary variables for social norms and perceived healthiness, as opposed to the 5- and 7-point continuous Likert scales, resulted in a loss of information. However, sensitivity analyses conducted using continuous variables based on all Likert scale levels showed similar patterns of results (data not shown), and figures showing the distribution of the response from the Likert scale level are provided in the Supplementary Figure S2 and S3.

Despite its previous evaluation for validity [42], the use of the self-reported Beverage Frequency Questionnaire is subject to measurement error, which may vary between different groups and over time as beverage markets are changing. Indeed, consumption of new beverage categories may not have been captured by this questionnaire, resulting in an underestimation of consumption, and the exclusion of coffee with sugar and specialty coffee in the sugary drink intake variable may have led to an underestimation of the prevalence of consumers, particularly in Australia, where ready-to-drink coffee is more commonly consumed [86]. Also, the Beverage Frequency Questionnaire reflects consumption over the past 7 days, which may improve the ability to assess consumption of less frequently consumed beverages, but also increases the difficulty of recall.

For the perceived healthiness variable, FOP “excess” warning labels were added to the sugary drink images of the Mexico questionnaire in 2020, in line with national regulations and to represent the current food environment in Mexico, which may have affected participants' perceptions. Nevertheless, the images shown in the survey were representative of the "real world" environment in which the participants are exposed to sugary drinks. This measure also only assessed one brand of beverages, and perceptions or actual healthiness may vary between various brands. Moreover, as the term "sugary drink" is more common, it was used in the social norm questions. However, the definition provided to participants prior to answering these questions referred to SSBs (containing added sugars), rather than sugary drinks (containing free sugars). Participants may have answered differently if the definition provided included 100% juice. This limitation highlights the critical need for clear, standardized definitions and nutrient profiling systems to accurately distinguish between “sugar”, “sweetened” and “sugar-sweetened' beverages”, particularly in policy discussions aimed at regulating these products [87]. Finally, the COVID-19 pandemic may have had an impact on participants’ responses, particularly on perceived social norms, as social interactions and social relationships changed significantly in 2020, although the directionality of this potential impact is unclear [88].

Conclusion

This study showed that perceived healthiness and social norms related to sugary drinks vary across countries and sociodemographic characteristics. Our findings highlighted several sugary drink categories, such as 100% juice, chocolate milk, and sports drinks, which are generally perceived as healthier by consumers and therefore warrant policy attention. In most cases, perceiving a beverage as unhealthy and agreeing with social norms discouraging sugary drink intake were associated with lower sugary drink consumption. This highlights the importance of policies that can influence perceptions of sugary drinks and cultural attitudes to ultimately contribute to a reduction in sugary drink consumption. Shifts in perceived healthiness and social norms observed in Mexico, a country actively addressing the issue of sugary drinks, suggest that these correlates are not fixed and that policies can play a role in shaping beliefs about these beverages.

Data availability

Data are available directly from the International Food Policy Study team on reasonable request (see: foodpolicystudy.com).

Abbreviations

AOR:

Adjusted odds ratio

FOP:

Front-of-pack labeling

IFPS:

International Food Policy Study

IRR:

Incident risk ratio

SSB:

Sugar-sweetened beverage

UK:

United Kingdom

US:

United-States

References

  1. Rana H, Mallet MC, Gonzalez A, Verreault MF, St-Pierre S. Free sugars consumption in Canada. Nutrients. 2021;13(5):1471.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Gupta A, Smithers LG, Braunack-Mayer A, Harford J. How much free sugar do australians consume? Findings from a national survey. ANZJPH. 2018;42(6):533–40.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Sánchez-Pimienta TG, Batis C, Lutter CK, Rivera JA. Sugar-sweetened beverages are the main sources of added sugar intake in the Mexican population. J Nutr. 2016;146(9):S1888–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Amoutzopoulos B, Steer T, Roberts C, Collins D, Page P. Free and added sugar consumption and adherence to guidelines: the UK National Diet and Nutrition Survey (2014/15–2015/16). Nutrients. 2020;12(2):393.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Kelly RK, Tong TYN, Watling CZ, Reynolds A, Piernas C, Schmidt JA, et al. Associations between types and sources of dietary carbohydrates and cardiovascular disease risk: a prospective cohort study of UK Biobank participants. BMC Med. 2023;21(1):34.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Drouin-Chartier JP, Zheng Y, Li Y, Malik V, Pan A, Bhupathiraju SN, et al. Changes in consumption of sugary beverages and artificially sweetened beverages and subsequent risk of type 2 diabetes: results from three large prospective U.S. cohorts of women and men. Diabetes Care. 2019;42(12):2181–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Chazelas E, Srour B, Desmetz E, Kesse-Guyot E, Julia C, Deschamps V, et al. Sugary drink consumption and risk of cancer: results from NutriNet-Santé prospective cohort. BMJ. 2019;366:l2408.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Moynihan P. Sugars and dental caries: evidence for setting a recommended threshold for intake. Adv Nutr. 2016;7(1):149–56.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Jones AC, Veerman JL, Hammond D. The health and economic impact of a tax on sugary drinks in Canada. Waterloo, ON: University of Waterloo; 2017.

  10. World Health Organization. Taxes on sugary drinks: why do it?. 2017:4. Available from: https://iris.who.int/bitstream/handle/10665/260253/WHO-NMH-PND-16.5Rev.1-eng.pdf?sequence=1&isAllowed=y. Cited 2024 Oct 24.

  11. Taillie LS, Reyes M, Colchero MA, Popkin B, Corvalán C. An evaluation of Chile’s Law of Food Labeling and Advertising on sugar-sweetened beverage purchases from 2015 to 2017: a before-and-after study. Basu S, editor. PLoS Med. 2020;17(2):e1003015.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. World Cancer Research Fund International. Building momentum: lessons on implementing a robust sugar sweetened beverage tax. 2018; Available from: https://www.wcrf.org/wp-content/uploads/2021/04/PPA-Building-Momentum-Report-WEB.pdf. Cited 2023 Apr 5.

  13. Farley TA, Halper HS, Carlin AM, Emmerson KM, Foster KN, Fertig AR. Mass media campaign to reduce consumption of sugar-sweetened beverages in a rural area of the United States. Am J Public Health. 2017;107(6):989–95.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Boles M, Adams A, Gredler A, Manhas S. Ability of a mass media campaign to influence knowledge, attitudes, and behaviors about sugary drinks and obesity. Prev Med. 2014;67:S40–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Czoli CD, Jones AC, Hammond D. Trends in sugary drinks in Canada, 2004 to 2015: a comparison of market sales and dietary intake data. Public Health Nutr. 2019;22(15):2723–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Jones AC, Kirkpatrick SI, Hammond D. Beverage consumption and energy intake among canadians: analyses of 2004 and 2015 national dietary intake data. Nutr J. 2019;18(1):60.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Shrapnel WS, Butcher BE. Sales of sugar-sweetened beverages in Australia: a trend analysis from 1997 to 2018. Nutrients. 2020;12(4):1016.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Euromonitor International. Soft drink in Mexico. 2024. Available from: https://www-portal-euromonitor-com. Cited 2024 Feb 6.

  19. Vercammen KA, Wyatt Koma J, Bleich SN. Trends in energy drink consumption among U.S. adolescents and adults, 2003–2016. Am J Prev Med. 2019;56(6):827–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Euromonitor International. Sports drinks. 2024. Available from: www-portal-euromonitor-com. Cited 2024 Feb 6.

  21. French S, Rosenberg M, Wood L, Maitland C, Shilton T, Pratt IS, et al. Soft drink consumption patterns among western australians. JNEB. 2013;45(6):525–32.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Barrett P, Imamura F, Brage S, Griffin SJ, Wareham NJ, Forouhi NG. Sociodemographic, lifestyle and behavioural factors associated with consumption of sweetened beverages among adults in Cambridgeshire, UK: the Fenland Study. Public Health Nutr. 2017;20(15):2766–77.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Lundeen EA, Park S, Pan L, Blanck HM. Daily intake of sugar-sweetened beverages among US adults in 9 States, by state and sociodemographic and behavioral characteristics, 2016. Prev Chronic Dis. 2018;15:180335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Park S, Pan L, Sherry B, Blanck HM. Consumption of sugar-sweetened beverages among US adults in 6 states: behavioral risk factor surveillance system, 2011. Prev Chronic Dis. 2014;11:130304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Warren C, Hobin E, Manuel DG, Anderson LN, Hammond D, Jessri M, et al. Socioeconomic position and consumption of sugary drinks, sugar-sweetened beverages and 100% juice among canadians: a cross-sectional analysis of the 2015 Canadian Community Health Survey–Nutrition. Can J Public Health. 2022;113(3):341–62.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Drewnowski A, Rehm CD. Socioeconomic gradient in consumption of whole fruit and 100% fruit juice among US children and adults. Nutr J. 2015;14(1):3.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Melnyk V, Carrillat FA, Melnyk V. The influence of social norms on consumer behavior: a meta-analysis. J Mark. 2022;86(3):98–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Melnyk V, van Herpen E, Jak S, van Trijp HCM. The mechanisms of social norms’ influence on consumer decision making: a meta-analysis. Z Psychol. 2019;227(1):4–17.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Fishbein M, Ajzen I. Predicting and changing behavior. Psychology Press; 2011. Available from: https://www.taylorfrancis.com/books/9781136874734.

  30. Fishbein M. A theory of reasoned action: some applications and implications. Nebr Symp Motiv. 1980;27:65–116.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Pelletier JE, Graham DJ, Laska MN. Social norms and dietary behaviors among young adults. Am J Hlth Behav. 2014;38(1):144–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Ball K, Jeffery RW, Abbott G, McNaughton SA, Crawford D. Is healthy behavior contagious: associations of social norms with physical activity and healthy eating. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2010;7(1):86.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Perkins JM, Perkins HW, Craig DW. Misperceived norms and personal sugar-sweetened beverage consumption and fruit and vegetable intake among students in the United States. Appetite. 2018;129:82–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Bélanger-Gravel A, Paquette MC, Espín-Espinoza A, Janezic I, Desroches S, De Wals P. The influence of social norms in the context of reducing sugar-sweetened beverages consumption. Public Health. 2022;213:28–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Provencher V, Polivy J, Herman CP. Perceived healthiness of food. If it’s healthy, you can eat more! Appetite. 2009;52(2):340–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Munsell CR, Harris JL, Sarda V, Schwartz MB. Parents’ beliefs about the healthfulness of sugary drink options: opportunities to address misperceptions. Public Health Nutr. 2016;19(1):46–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Bucher T, Siegrist M. Children’s and parents’ health perception of different soft drinks. Br J Nutr. 2015;113(3):526–35.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Kim H, House LA. Linking consumer health perceptions to consumption of nonalcoholic beverages. Agric Resour econ rev. 2014;43(1):1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Hammond D, Vanderlee L, White CM, Acton RB, White M, Roberto CA, et al. The conceptual Framework for the International Food Policy Study: evaluating the population-level impact of food policy. J Nutr. 2022;152(Supplement1):S1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. International Food Policy Study. Methods. Available from: https://foodpolicystudy.com/methods/. Cited 2023 Oct 2.

  41. Diario Oficial de la Federación. Modificacion a la Norma Oficial Mexicana NOM-051-SCFI/SSA1-2010. Mexico. 2020. Available from: https://www.dof.gob.mx/2020/SEECO/NOM_051.pdf. Cited 2024 Mar 8.

  42. Vanderlee L, Reid JL, White CM, Hobin EP, Acton RB, Jones AC, et al. Evaluation of the online Beverage frequency questionnaire (BFQ). Nutr J. 2018;17(1):73.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Litwin H, Sapir EV. Perceived income adequacy among older adults in 12 countries: findings from the survey of health, ageing, and retirement in Europe. Gerontologist. 2009;49(3):397–406.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Sullivan GM, Feinn R. Using effect size—or why the P value is not enough. J Grad Med Educ. 2012;4(3):279–82.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Miller C, Ettridge K, Wakefield M, Pettigrew S, Coveney J, Roder D, et al. An in-depth exploration of knowledge and beliefs associated with soda and diet soda consumption. Nutrients. 2020;12(9):2841.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Brownbill AL, Braunack-Mayer AJ, Miller CL. What makes a beverage healthy? A qualitative study of young adults’ conceptualisation of sugar-containing beverage healthfulness. Appetite. 2020;150:104675.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Sylvetsky AC, Hoang ST, Visek AJ, Halberg SE, Smith M, Salahmand Y, et al. Beverage characteristics perceived as healthy among hispanic and African-American parents of young children. JAND. 2022;122(6):1158–67.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Imamura F, O’Connor L, Ye Z, Mursu J, Hayashino Y, Bhupathiraju SN, et al. Consumption of sugar sweetened beverages, artificially sweetened beverages, and fruit juice and incidence of type 2 diabetes: systematic review, meta-analysis, and estimation of population attributable fraction. BMJ. 2015;351:h3576.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Nguyen M, Jarvis SE, Chiavaroli L, Mejia SB, Zurbau A, Khan TA, et al. Consumption of 100% fruit Juice and body weight in children and adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2024;178(3):237–46.

  50. Herforth A, Arimond M, Álvarez-Sánchez C, Coates J, Christianson K, Muehlhoff E. A global review of food-based dietary guidelines. Adv Nutr. 2019;10(4):590–605.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Global Food Research Program. UNC-Chapel Hill. 2023. Taxes on unhealthy foods and beverages. Available from: https://www.globalfoodresearchprogram.org/resource/taxes-on-unhealthy-foods-and-beverages/. Cited 2024 Mar 21.

  52. Frank T, Ng SW, Miles DR, Swart EC. Applying and comparing various nutrient profiling models against the packaged food supply in South Africa. Public Health Nutr. 2022;25(8):2296–307.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Jáuregui A, White CM, Vanderlee L, Hall MG, Contreras-Manzano A, Nieto C, et al. Impact of front-of-pack labels on the perceived healthfulness of a sweetened fruit drink: a randomised experiment in five countries. Public Health Nutr. 2022;25(4):1094–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Treasury HM. GOV.UK. 2018. Soft Drinks Industry Levy comes into effect. Available from: https://www.gov.uk/government/news/soft-drinks-industry-levy-comes-into-effect. Cited 2023 Mar 13.

  55. Scarborough P, Adhikari V, Harrington RA, Elhussein A, Briggs A, Rayner M, et al. Impact of the announcement and implementation of the UK Soft drinks Industry Levy on sugar content, price, product size and number of available soft drinks in the UK, 2015-19: a controlled interrupted time series analysis. Popkin BM, editor. PLoS Med. 2020;17(2):e1003025.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. LiveLighter. Drink healthier. 2024. Available from: https://livelighter.com.au/eating-well/healthy-eating/sugary-drinks. Cited 2024 Apr 3.

  57. Cancer Council Victoria. Rethink sugary drink. 2024. Available from: https://www.rethinksugarydrink.org.au/. Cited 2024 Oct 23.

  58. Koch F, Hoffmann I, Claupein E. Types of nutrition knowledge, their socio-demographic determinants and their association with food consumption: results of the NEMONIT study. Front Nutr. 2021;8:630014.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Konttinen H, Halmesvaara O, Fogelholm M, Saarijärvi H, Nevalainen J, Erkkola M. Sociodemographic differences in motives for food selection: results from the LoCard cross-sectional survey. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2021;18(1):71.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Sylvetsky AC, Visek AJ, Halberg S, Rhee DK, Ongaro Z, Essel KD, et al. Beyond taste and easy access: physical, cognitive, interpersonal, and emotional reasons for sugary drink consumption among children and adolescents. Appetite. 2020;155:104826.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Harris JL, Romo-Palafox MJ, Choi YY, Kibwana, A. Children’s Drink FACTS 2019: Sales, nutrition, and marketing of children’s drinks. UConn Rudd Center for Food Policy & Obesity; 2019. Available from: https://uconnruddcenter.org/wp-content/uploads/sites/2909/2020/09/FACTS2019-1.pdf.

  62. Hennessy M, Bleakley A, Piotrowski JT, Mallya G, Jordan A. Sugar-sweetened beverage consumption by adult caregivers and their children: the role of drink features and advertising exposure. Health Educ Behav. 2015;42(5):677–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Choi YY, Jensen ML, Fleming-Milici F, Harris JL. Caregivers’ provision of sweetened fruit-flavoured drinks to young children: importance of perceived product attributes and differences by socio-demographic and behavioural characteristics. Public Health Nutr. 2022;25(8):2308–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Hall MG, Lazard AJ, Grummon AH, Mendel JR, Taillie LS. The impact of front-of-package claims, fruit images, and health warnings on consumers’ perceptions of sugar-sweetened fruit drinks: three randomized experiments. Prev Med. 2020;132:105998.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  65. Bleakley A, Ellithorpe ME, Jordan AB, Hennessy M, Stevens R. A content analysis of sports and energy drink advertising. Appetite. 2022;174:106010.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Fleming-Milici F, Phaneuf L, Harris JL. Marketing of sugar-sweetened children’s drinks and parents’ misperceptions about benefits for young children. Matern Child Nutr. 2022;18(3):e13338.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Cuy Castellanos D, Miller ME. Exploration of sugar-sweetened beverage intake in a Latinx population. JNEB. 2020;52(3):281–9.

    Google Scholar 

  68. Théodore F, Bonvecchio A, Blanco I, Irizarry L, Nava A. Significados culturalmente construidos para el consumo de bebidas azucaradas entre escolares de la Ciudad de México. Rev Panam Salud Públ. 2011;30(4). Available from: https://iris.paho.org/handle/10665.2/9516.

  69. Mayer J, Verhoeven AAC, Dornic Q, Hanzouli H, Seksek I, Guelinckx I, et al. Understanding attitudes to change to healthier hydration habits: the case of high sugar: low water drinkers in Mexico. Ann Nutr Metab. 2020;76(Suppl 1):43–52.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Kwon J, Cameron AJ, Hammond D, White CM, Vanderlee L, Bhawra J, et al. A multi-country survey of public support for food policies to promote healthy diets: findings from the International Food Policy Study. BMC Public Health. 2019;19(1):1205.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Stamkou E, Van Kleef GA, Homan AC, Gelfand MJ, Van De Vijver FJR, Van Egmond MC, et al. Cultural collectivism and tightness moderate responses to norm violators: effects on power perception, moral emotions, and leader support. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2019;45(6):947–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Barquera S, Campos I, Rivera JA. Mexico attempts to tackle obesity: the process, results, push backs and future challenges. Obes Rev. 2013;14(S2):69–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Alienza por la salud alimentaria. 2024. Available from: https://alianzasalud.org.mx/. Cited 2024 Feb 9.

  74. El poder del Consumidor. La Alianza por la Salud Alimentaria —de la que somos parte— denuncia a Concamin de intentar suspender publicación de nueva norma de etiquetado. 2020. Available from: https://elpoderdelconsumidor.org/2020/03/la-alianza-por-la-salud-alimentaria-de-la-que-somos-parte-denuncia-a-concamin-de-intentar-suspender-publicacion-de-nueva-norma-de-etiquetado/. Cited 2023 Oct 14.

  75. Goodman S, Armendariz GC, Corkum A, Arellano L, Jáuregui A, Keeble M, et al. Recall of government healthy eating campaigns by consumers in five countries. Public Health Nutr. 2021;24(13):3986–4000.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  76. Hattersley L, Irwin M, King L, Allman-Farinelli M. Determinants and patterns of soft drink consumption in young adults: a qualitative analysis. Public Health Nutr. 2009;12(10):1816–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Lee SH, Park S, Lehman TC, Ledsky R, Blanck HM. Occasions, locations, and reasons for consuming sugar-sweetened beverages among U.S. adults. Nutrients. 2023;15(4):920.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  78. Miller C, Braunack-Mayer A, Wakefield M, Roder D, O’Dea K, Dono J, et al. When we were young, it really was a treat; now sugar is just the norm every day a qualitative study of parents’ and young adults’ perceptions and consumption of sugary drinks. Health Promot J Austral. 2020;31(1):47–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Lara-Castor L, Micha R, Cudhea F, Miller V, Shi P, Zhang J, et al. Sugar-sweetened beverage intakes among adults between 1990 and 2018 in 185 countries. Nat Commun. 2023;14(1):5957.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  80. Pedroza-Tobias A, Crosbie E, Mialon M, Carriedo A, Schmidt LA. Food and beverage industry interference in science and policy: efforts to block soda tax implementation in Mexico and prevent international diffusion. BMJ Glob Health. 2021;6(8):e005662.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  81. Rosas CE, Gregorio-Pascual P, Driver R, Martinez A, Price SL, Lopez C, et al. Effects of social norms information and self-affirmation on sugar-sweetened beverage consumption intentions and behaviors. Basic Appl Soc Psychol. 2017;39(2):112–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Malone RE, Grundy Q, Bero LA. Tobacco industry denormalisation as a tobacco control intervention: a review. Tob Control. 2012;21(2):162–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Vanderlee L, White CM, Kirkpatrick SI, Rynard VL, Jáuregui A, Adams J, et al. Nonalcoholic and alcoholic beverage intakes by adults across 5 upper-middle- and high-income countries. J Nutr. 2021;151(1):140–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Government of Canada. Front-of-package nutrition labelling. 2023. Available from: https://www.canada.ca/en/health-canada/services/food-labelling-changes/front-package.html. Cited 2023 Mar 13.

  85. US Food & drug administration. Front-of-Package Nutrition Labeling. 2024. Available from: https://www.fda.gov/food/nutrition-food-labeling-and-critical-foods/front-package-nutrition-labeling#:~:text=The%20National%20Strategy%20highlights%20that,build%20a%20healthy%20eating%20pattern.

  86. Euromonitor International. Ready-to-drink coffee in Australia. 2024. Available from: www-portal-euromonitor-com. Cited 2024 Feb 6.

  87. Sousa A, Sych J, Rohrmann S, Faeh D. The importance of sweet beverage definitions when targeting health policies—the case of Switzerland. Nutrients. 2020;12(7):1976.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  88. Mathieu E, Ritchie H, Rodés-Guirao L, Appel C, Gavrilov D, Giattino H et al. Our world in data. 2020. Policy responses to the coronavirus pandemic. Available from: https://ourworldindata.org/policy-responses-covid. Cited 2023 Dec 6.

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the statisticians Pierre Gagnon and Clara-Jane Rhéaume for analytical support and the participants for the time taken to complete the study.

Funding

Funding for this project was provided by a Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) Project Grant (PJT162167), with additional support for the adult survey from the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Disorders of the National Institutes of Health (R01 DK128967), from the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR13059), from the Public Health Agency of Canada (PHAC) and from a CIHR-PHAC Applied Public Health Chair. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the Canadian Institutes for Health Research, or the National Institutes of Health. VDL received funding from the Fonds de recherche du Québec-Santé. LV is supported by a Canada Research Chair. JA was supported by the Medical Research Council [grant number MC_UU_00006/7].

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

LV and VDL conceived the idea for the paper. VDL analyzed the data, drafted the manuscript, and was supervised by LV. All authors read, critically revised, and approved the final manuscript. DH conceived the idea for the IFPS and DH, LV, JFT, SIK, GS, AJ, SB, and JA secured funding. DH, LV, and CMW developed the first draft of the survey.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lana Vanderlee.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study received ethics approval from the University of Waterloo Research Ethics Committee (ORE #30828) and Université Laval Ethics Committee (#2021 − 318 Phase II A-2 R-2 / 11-12-2023). All participants provided consent prior to completing the survey.

Consent for publication

Not required.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

12937_2024_1063_MOESM1_ESM.docx

Additional files 1. Figure S1: Images shown to participants for questions on perceived healthiness, by country. Figure S2: Weighted percentages of participants reporting each level of the perceived healthiness Likert scale by beverage category, across all countries and years. Figure S3: Weighted percentages of participants reporting each level of the Likert scale for the descriptive and injunctive norms, across all countries and years.

Additional files 2. Tables S1 to S7: Results for perceived healthiness of other beverages.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Drolet-Labelle, V., White, C.M., Adams, J. et al. Perceived healthiness of sugary drinks and related social norms among adults in five countries: evidence from the International Food Policy Study. Nutr J 24, 19 (2025). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12937-024-01063-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12937-024-01063-8

Keywords